logo

64 pages 2 hours read

Philip G. Zimbardo

The Lucifer Effect: Understanding How Good People Turn Evil

Nonfiction | Book | Adult | Published in 2007

A modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.

Chapters 10-11Chapter Summaries & Analyses

Chapter 10 Summary: “The SPE’s Meaning and Messages: The Alchemy of Character Transformations”

The Stanford Prison Experiment assesses “the extent to which the external features of an institutional setting [can] override the internal dispositions of the actors in that environment” (195). The experiment has become famous for exposing that bad systems and situations can cause good people to behave in evil ways alien to their nature. The loss of personal identity, the subjection to arbitrary continual control, the loss of privacy, and the sleep deprivation that the prisoners experienced during the experiment all caused what is now known as “learned helplessness” (195). Because the prisoners were screened prior to the experiment to ensure they did not import any pathologies, Zimbardo concludes that all “pathologies were elicited by the set of situational forces constantly impinging upon them in this prisonlike setting” (197).

Zimbardo identifies problems present in data analysis of the Stanford Prison Experiment: the sample size is small, video recordings are selective, causal directions are uncertain, and the experiment has no control group. The only independent variable is “the treatment of guard-versus-prisoner status” (197). Zimbardo concludes, however, that clear patterns emerge.

Prior to the experiment, subjects were evaluated on three different measures: “the F-Scale of authoritarianism, the Machiavellian Scale of interpersonal manipulation strategies, and the Comrey Personality Scales” (198). Prisoners and guard groups had obtained similar F-Scale scores. Prisoners who lasted the duration of the study had scored higher on the F-Scale than those who were sent home early. The guard group had a slightly higher Machiavellian Scale score than the prisoners, but score differences among participants did not affect subjects’ behavior. Subjects exhibited several different individual behaviors that corresponded to their Comrey Personality Scale, but Zimbardo deems individual differences insignificant. Significantly, there were “no personality precursors for the difference between the four meanest guards and the others who were less abusive” (200).

In their mood adjective self-reports, prisoners described themselves much more negatively than the guards, while the guards described themselves as more negative than positive. Prisoner scores were double the guards’ score on activity-passivity. Prisoners who left the experiment early scored higher on depression and unhappiness, but their scores changed to elevated positivity soon after they were released: “This return to normality seems to reflect the ‘situational specificity’ of the depression and stress reactions these students experienced while playing their unusual roles” (201).

Analysis of the video recordings reveals “an excess of negative, hostile interactions between the guards and prisoners” (202). Videos also show that guards escalated their harassment over time, and prisoners correspondingly behaved worse and worse. Analysis of the audio recordings exposes that guards had as much negative outlook and negative self-regard as prisoners. Audio recordings also show that 90 percent of conversations among prisoners related solely to prison issues: “The prisoner role dominated all expressions of individual character. The prison setting dominated their outlook and concerns—forcing them into an expanded present temporal orientation” (204).

Consequently, prisoners slowly grew to accept the negative experiences that the guards imposed on them. Zimbardo classifies half of all the experiment’s private prisoner interactions as lacking mutual support and cooperation, and 85 percent of prisoner statements regarding fellow prisoners as “uncomplimentary and depreciating” (205). He explains that the prison setting that served to humiliate the prisoners impacted the prisoners’ impressions of each other. The prisoners became one with their enemy, and this process prevented realistic appraisals of the situation, inhibited action against their oppressors and their ability to cope, and quashed their own rebellion.

Zimbardo claims that the Stanford Prison Experiment’s basic protocol enabled its success, arguing that, while the prison setting is artificial, “all research is ‘artificial,’ being only an imitation of its real-world analogue” (206). Zimbardo argues that despite the experiment’s artificiality, its results have considerable generalizability because it presents psychological features central to the “prison experience.” The experiment shows that possessing a psychology of individual invulnerability to situational forces compromises individuals by making them insufficiently vigilant to situational forces. Zimbardo argues that people can avoid, prevent, challenge, and change negative situational forces by recognizing their power.

The experiment showed Zimbardo that situational power is strongest in unfamiliar settings. Every analysis should begin situationally and yield to dispositional analysis only when the situational analysis does not suffice to explain behavior. The experiment also exposes “the power of rules” (212): a simple and formal method of controlling informal complex behavior. Some societal rules are necessary, but many allow the rule makers and enforcers to dominate, as the prison experiment proves. Further, the experiment shows anonymity’s power to induce persons to behave in antisocial ways, especially in a setting which grants permission for such actions: “When all members of a group of individuals are in a deindividuated state, their mental functioning changes: they live in an expanded-present moment that makes past and future distant and irrelevant” (219).

Perhaps the most profound observations from the experiment are the ones that related to the dehumanizing attitudes the experiment conferred on participants. Dehumanization occurs when individuals believe “others” do not have the same feelings, thoughts, values, and life purposes as they do. Human relationships are subjective, personal, and emotional, while dehumanized relationships are objectifying, analytical, and nonempathetic. Consequently, the prison experiment was able to dehumanize the participants in the same way a real prison dehumanizes its prisoners. Lacking personal space and a sense of true community, inmates are isolated and anonymous; the guards determined how they would react to each other, so “[t]ender, caring emotions were absent among both guards and prisoners after only a few days” (223).

Systems create what Zimbardo calls ‘Situations’ by providing the institutional support, authority, and resources that Situations need to operate. Determining who or what controls a System is difficult in complex organizations, such as correctional facilities, megacorporations, and governmental entities. According to Zimbardo, “System Power involves authorization or institutionalized permission to behave in prescribed ways or to forbid and punish actions that are contrary to them” (226). New ways of behaving lead to new norms and ethics, judged by new authority figures unique to the System. 

Chapter 11 Summary: “The SPE: Ethics and Extensions”

Zimbardo reasons that humans possess a fundamental need to belong, which is borne from desires to associate, to cooperate, and to accept group norms. However, outside forces can pervert this desire of belonging into excessive conformity, compliance, and exclusionary hostility. Similarly, the human need for autonomy and control can transform into excessive power and domination. Human needs—like the need for consistency and rationality, the need to know and understand one’s environment and relationship to it, and the need for stimulation—can engender both positivity and negativity. Treating evil as systemic rather than individual does not excuse evil actions, and individuals must “be held responsible and legally accountable for their complicity and crimes” (231). He concedes that situational and systemic causal factors must be accounted for in punishment.

Zimbardo questions the ethics of the experiment. He explains that every action that intervenes in another’s life raises ethical considerations, even when intentions are good. The actor’s subjective values determine the ethics of such actions. Ethics can be “absolute” or “relative.” Absolute ethical standards are determined by a higher-order moral principle that is not dependent on the conditions in which the standards are applied. As such, “no extenuating circumstances can justify an abrogation of the ethical standard” (233).

Under this absolute definition of ethics, Zimbardo concludes that the Stanford Prison Experiment was unethical for two reasons. Firstly, in relation to both the subjects and their family members, “human beings did suffer considerable anguish” (233) and secondly, the research team failed to terminate the experiment abruptly upon observing such signs of anguish. Zimbardo qualifies this self-evaluation with the facts that no legal charges were filed, and the American Psychological Association evaluated the experiment and “determined that the existing ethical guidelines had been followed” (235).

Most psychological research adheres to utilitarian, or relative ethics: “When an ethical principle admits of contingent applications, its standard is relative and it is to be judged on pragmatic criteria weighted according to utilitarian principles” (235). Under this criterion, a social scientist must balance what is necessary for research and what is necessary for the ethical treatment of research subjects. Zimbardo argues that the Stanford Prison Experiment was ethical under the criteria of relative ethics for many reasons: the creators consulted Stanford University’s legal counsel and student health department, and they executed an “informed consent” statement that detailed for subjects the significant privacy and civil rights considerations of participation. Zimbardo emphasizes that the subjects were not deceived, his team frequently reminded guards not to physically abuse prisoners, the “prison was open to inspection by outsiders” (236), and the team extensively debriefed the subjects both immediately following the experiment and subsequently.

Zimbardo further explains ethics in psychological experiments. Research must posit a scientific, medicinal, and/or societal gain which outweighs the cost to subjects. Zimbardo challenges this idea because the costs are immediate, while gains are anticipatory, probable, distant, and possibly unrealizable, depending on the experiment’s results. Zimbardo criticizes this rubric further because it fails to account for “the net gain to the participants” (238). He proceeds to state several examples of Stanford Prison Experiment subjects benefitting later in life from their experience in the experiment: prisoner Doug-8612 earned his PhD in psychology because of his experience in the experiment and has been a forensic psychologist in the California corrections systems for more than 20 years; guard Hellman states, “[T]he memory of how I fell so deeply into my role that I was blind to the suffering of others serves today as a cautionary tale, and I think carefully about how I treat people” (240). Zimbardo continues to recount positive effects of the experiment on other subjects and administrators, including his own work on shyness as a self-imposed prison, time perspective biases, and madness in normal people. Zimbardo reaches the following groundbreaking conclusion: “the seeds of madness can be planted in anyone’s backyard and will grow in response to transient psychological perturbations in the course of the lifetime of ordinary experience” (246).

Zimbardo further argues that he spread his experiment’s positive impact by preaching its gospel in every public forum available, including a major documentary and a highly-trafficked website. However, Zimbardo also laments the utilization of his research by the United States government to maximize their ability to mistreat prisoners.

Chapters 10-11 Analysis

Zimbardo explains that his team screened subjects for the Stanford Prison Experiment and that those they selected all occupied normal ranges when tested by various psychological exams; thus, they did not bring their own pathologies into the experiment. He analyzes the experiment’s psychological effects on some of the subjects, highlighting the extreme negativity felt by prisoners towards themselves and their situation, and the way in which the role completely consumes their identities. This phenomenon happens in the context of their groups, emphasizing the theme of conformity versus individualism.

Zimbardo extrapolates from his data that situational power is strongest in unfamiliar settings, that the worst thing persons can do is dehumanize other persons, and that Systems shape Situations by providing institutional support, authority, and resources which permit Situations to operate. He also argues that the SPE’s artificiality does not preclude its real-life applicability, stating that all research is artificial, but properly conducted research can influence real-world results.

Zimbardo addresses ethical concerns in the Stanford Prison Experiment. Though he notes that under an absolute ethical model, his experiment is unethical, because such a model does not permit any harm to subjects for any reason, he balances this explanation with another point of view. Zimbardo explains that psychological research is conducted under utilitarian, or relativist, ethics. Zimbardo argues that under a relativist ethical approach, his experiment is ethical for several reasons: it complies with many legal and ethical formalities, such as providing informed consent and obtaining approval from the University’s legal department, and the long-term positive societal effect of the study outweighs the harm to participants, which was temporary. (Everyone’s psychological condition improved shortly after the experiment terminated and there have been no negative long-term psychological effects among subjects.) Zimbardo provides examples of many positive long-term effects of the study among subjects and researchers. He ensures that the positive effects of his study are widespread by promoting the research publicly in many global forums, including the internet, congressional hearings, television, courtrooms, and public speeches. This discussion of the ethics involved in the experiment impact the thematic significance of authority and the abuse of power

blurred text
blurred text
blurred text
blurred text