37 pages • 1 hour read
Malcolm GladwellA modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.
Summary
Chapter Summaries & Analyses
Key Figures
Themes
Index of Terms
Important Quotes
Essay Topics
Tools
Gladwell’s goal in Part 3 is to address his second puzzle: Why do we often misjudge strangers after meeting them? Why does the extra information we glean from face-to-face interaction sometimes hurt, and not help, our ability to read them? He begins by looking at the work of psychologist Jennifer Fugate, who is an expert in a system called FACS (Facial Action Coding System). FACS involves assigning unique numbers to different muscular movements in the face, followed by a letter indicating the intensity of each movement.
Gladwell asked Fugate to analyze a brief segment from an episode of the TV show Friends. Her analysis showed that the actors consistently used facial expressions with moderate to high intensity, and that their facial expressions were exactly what you would expect for the emotions a character was feeling—a jaw drop when a character was surprised, a furrowed brow when a character was angry, and so on. The actors’ facial expressions were transparent—they provided an accurate window into the way the characters felt on the inside. Gladwell claims that this notion of transparency is the second tool that we use to make sense of strangers. When we haven’t had the time to really get to know a person, we rely on their outward behavior and demeanor to get a sense of what the person is thinking or feeling.
Real life is not like Friends, however. Social scientists Sergio Jarillo and Carlos Crivelli did a study on how we match emotions to facial expressions. Their participants from Spain completed the task as expected (“happy” was matched to the smiling face, “angry” to the scowling face, and so on), but they got completely different results when they repeated the experiment with participants from the Trobriand Islands in Papua New Guinea. While we all feel the same emotions on the inside, we do not all show our emotions on the outside in the same way.
Research by psychologists Achim Schützwohl and Rainer Reisenzein has further shown that we don’t always make the facial expressions we think we do. They set up a study designed to genuinely surprise their participants. Nearly all the participants were convinced afterwards that their faces had shown the telltale signs of surprise—wide eyes, raised eyebrows, and dropped jaws—but in reality, the great majority of them had very different expressions, such as knitted eyebrows. Many of the associations we’ve formed between emotions and facial expressions are mere stereotypes we’ve learned from books and TV.
Transparency, Gladwell claims, is a myth. When interacting with friends and family, we have already learned their idiosyncrasies and how they personally express their emotions. With strangers, however, all we have to rely on are highly unreliable stereotypes. Looking someone in the face may do little more than mislead us, but at the same time we need to meet with people face-to-face for our interactions to feel human. Like default-to-truth, Gladwell concludes that the assumption of transparency is “deeply flawed” but a social necessity all the same.
A student named Meredith Kercher was murdered in the Italian city of Perugia in 2007. While there was significant evidence indicating that a man named Rudy Guede was responsible for the murder, the investigation instead focused on Kercher’s roommate, Amanda Knox. The police had no physical evidence implicating Knox but rather based their suspicions on her unusual behavior following the crime. They made the mistake of assuming that people are transparent.
While we tend to default to truth when judging a person’s honesty, their demeanor can also play an important role. If someone acts confident, we are more inclined to believe they are telling the truth. If someone acts nervous and rambles, we are more inclined to believe they are lying. The problem is that these behaviors are simply stereotypes of how honest and dishonest people behave. Some people are “matched,” such as a person who fidgets and blushes when they lie, but other people are “mismatched.” Bernie Madoff and Adolf Hitler, for instance, were liars who had the demeanors of honest people.
Gladwell argues that Knox was also mismatched. Rather than appearing sad in the wake of Kercher’s death, Knox came across as indifferent, flippant, and angry at times. She did not behave the way the police thought grieving people ought to behave, and as a result she spent years in prison for a crime she did not commit. Knox is far from the only person to suffer for being different. Cases like hers happen all the time. Research by Tim Levine has indicated that trained interrogators are particularly good at spotting when “matched” people are being honest or dishonest, but they perform extraordinarily badly on “mismatched” people.
In 2015, two people attested that they witnessed a student named Brock Turner raping an unconscious woman at Stanford University. Both Turner and the victim (known by the alias Emily Doe) were drunk when that they met, but the case rested on whether Doe had been so drunk that she was incapable of consenting when Turner initiated sex with her. He was ultimately found guilty and sentenced to six months in prison. Gladwell notes that cases of sexual assault are particularly complex because of the widely varying conceptions of what does or does not constitute consent. Alcohol complicates matters even further. Reading another person’s intentions is difficult enough when sober but becomes all the more difficult when inebriated.
The psychologists Claude Steele and Robert Josephs argue that alcohol causes myopia. This involves “narrow[ing] our emotional and mental fields of vision” so that we are far more focused on the here and now (207), while any long-term considerations fade into the background. By removing those constraints on our behavior, Gladwell believes that alcohol temporarily “obliterates our true self” (210). This does not necessarily mean that it is a disinhibiting agent—a study of a small Bolivian community found that although community members drank heavily and frequently, their town suffered from none of the social ills typically associated with alcohol. Rather, alcohol-induced myopia means that we are reshaped according to our surroundings. The Bolivian community did not have problems because they drank in a controlled and predictable environment.
When the surroundings are the “hypersexualized chaos of a frat party” (232), however, alcohol can transform people’s behavior for the worse. A sexually aggressive person may control their impulses when sober but not when alcohol is thrown into the mix. Gladwell believes that until we recognize the fundamental role alcohol plays in many sexual assault cases, the pain and suffering experienced by Emily Doe will be repeated again and again.
While Part 2 explored our default assumption that strangers are honest, Part 3 examines a second assumption we make: that strangers are transparent. This is not a terribly effective strategy for understanding strangers, as Gladwell endeavors to show.
Most of Chapter 6 is dedicated to social science research on facial expressions. Not only do people have greatly varying cultural norms when it comes to expressing emotions, but we aren’t always aware of how we express our emotions ourselves. We’ve been led to believe that we can tell what a person is thinking or feeling by studying their face or body language, but that simply isn’t the case. While default-to-truth is a reasonable assumption to make given that most people are honest, the same does not hold true of the notion of transparency. Transparency is instead based on stereotypes that very frequently do not hold true.
In these chapters, it becomes evident that the assumption of transparency can interact with our inclination to default to truth. Amanda Knox’s case indeed seems to be the polar opposite of everything that Gladwell explored in the previous section. Rather than defaulting to truth, people assumed the worst about Knox. They assumed she was cold, conniving, dishonest, and promiscuous. The media ran scandalous stories of her killing Kercher in a sex game gone wrong. Even though Knox was exonerated in 2015, there are still those who believe she is guilty.
Knox was thus believed to be a killer despite the lack of evidence against her, yet people such as Sandusky or Madoff were given the benefit of the doubt. The key difference between them lies in their behavior. Knox was presumed guilty because society penalizes “weirdness.” Her friends and family knew that she was a bit awkward and quirky, but people are less accepting when a stranger’s behavior does not fall within social norms. Sandusky, on the other hand, came across as a fun-loving man who genuinely cared about children. Madoff was charismatic and confident. Default-to-truth is a tendency that many of us have, but it is not a hard-and-fast rule. Our impressions of people based on their outward demeanor also play an important role.
Misreading a person can have disastrous consequences. It can mean sending the wrong person to prison, or it can mean believing a person has consented to sex when they haven’t. While it is worth knowing that our strategies for making sense of strangers are unreliable, the answer is not necessarily to do away with them. Near the end of Chapter 6, Gladwell briefly revisits his interview with Judge Solomon (previously mentioned in Chapter 2). He asks, given how much we misread people, wouldn’t it be better if judges never even saw the defendants? Judge Solomon’s response to this question shows why that wouldn’t work. We need to look people in the face to feel a connection with them—to see them as human beings with their own thoughts, feelings, and experiences. Solomon didn’t want to make a decision regarding someone’s freedom without seeing the human being that he was affecting.
Trying to read people’s faces may lead to us misunderstanding them, but taking away the stranger’s face could potentially lead us to dehumanize them. Just as we must maintain our propensity to default to truth even if it sometimes leads us astray, we also need to maintain face-to-face interactions with people even if we often misread them.
By Malcolm Gladwell